Fostering Inter-Connectedness With Animals And Nature: The Developmental 
Benefits For Children. 

By Gail F. Melson, Ph.D. Originally published in People, 
Animals, Environment, Fall 1990, pps 15-17.

Gail Melson is an Associate Professor In the Department of Child Development and 
Family Studies at Purdue University. 

Experiences of interconnectedness with animals and with nature may be an 
important context within which more nurturing children may grow to be more 
nurturing adults. I first will briefly underline the importance of studying this 
aspect of development and suggest some reasons why developmental psychologists 
and others who study children's development have neglected this topic. Using 
evidence from the relatively recent research in this area, I will trace out a 
research and theory agenda for studying ways in which nurturance may be fostered 
throughout the child's development, with particular attention to 
inter-connectedness with animals and with nature. Finally, I'll suggest some 
other developmental areas that may be affected by such inter-connectedness. 

Importance Of The Development Of Nurturance
By nurturance, we mean, "fostering the developmental needs of another through 
provision of care, guidance and protection." We often think of caregiving as 
synonymous with nurturance, but note that this definition is broader, since 
responding to another's needs may involve a range of behaviors beyond 
caregiving. Moreover, caregiving may be intrusive, controlling and unresponsive 
to the recipient's needs. The second point about this definition is that it is 
explicitly developmental. We believe that nurturance develops; like other 
behaviors, it has the potential to become more skilled, differentiated and 
appropriate. 
The importance of nurturance may be underscored by thinking of a number of 
societal trends which are accelerating into the next century. Increased 
longevity and medical advances mean we anticipate larger populations requiring 
more long-term care; the norm of the dual employed couple and the single parent 
head of household mean more children needing care by nonparental figures; the 
integration of people with disabilities into the mainstream of education and 
employment means we must incorporate greater sensitivity to individual 
developmental needs within the school and the workplace; and the conservative 
revolt against government provision of services and the taxes to pay for them 
means a greater reliance on voluntary citizen efforts. All these trends are 
occurring at the same time, as women are less available to perform traditional 
caregiving and volunteer roles. Because the need for nurturers is increasing at 
the same time as the traditional pool is shrinking, it is important to 
understand how nurturing others develops during childhood and how we can foster 
such development. 
Children are traditionally thought of as recipients of nurturance from parents 
and other caregivers, not as nurturers themselves. Within our culture, there is 
emphasis on the individual ego or self as the unit of development, as reflected 
in the great psychological theories of development of Freud and Erikson, or in 
theories of human needs, such as Maslow's in which self-actualization is the 
highest order of human need. Historically, North American culture places a high 
premium on individuality, from our emphasis on individual liberties and privacy 
to our history of individual risk-taking through immigration to this country 
from other lands and to the West from the historic Eastern seaboard. 
As a result, child-rearing values within our society tend to focus on the need 
to foster independence, initiative and assertion. The need for children to care 
for others, to learn how to contribute to the well-being of others outside of 
themselves is not well recognized by parents or by educators. However, new 
research on the development of attachment and its long-term effects challenge 
this view that parenting springs full blown upon the birth of one's infant. This 
research documents that early relationships create an "internal working model" 
or mental representation of parenting and being parented, which is carried 
forward in time and place to form a template for other relationships. This idea 
is now being fruitfully used to explain the intergenerational transmission of 
abuse. This line of research challenges us to look back at children as a sort of 
savings account into which nurturing and being nurtured experiences are stored 
for later withdrawal. 
Other lines of research within psychology and education are adding fuel to the 
argument that nurturance develops during childhood. Researchers on infancy have 
documented the social, interactive nature of the infant from birth on and shown 
that all individualism is rooted in the establishment of secure interpersonal 
attachments. Within education, cooperative learning and supportive contexts as 
facilitators of individual achievement have been well documented. We have 
evidence that the social networks of children are important supports during 
stress, as social networks are for adults. Thus, from a number of research 
traditions, we are gradually coming to a new realization: nurturance develops 
during childhood, and this development is important both for optimal childhood 
functioning and for later adult functioning. Now that we have argued for the 
importance of nurturance as a developmental construct, let's briefly describe 
why connections to animals and nature are good places to study this development. 
First, pets are a common feature of children's households. Demographic surveys 
suggest pets are more common in households with children than in childless 
households, and parents report acquiring pets because of the presumed 
developmental benefits to children. Interviews with parents and children 
indicate that when pets are in a household, they generally assume great 
emotional significance, often being considered family members. 
Secondly, domestic animals and pets as well as plants depend for their 
development on the care provided by the humans responsible for them. They signal 
their development needs in fairly clear ways, and they provide immediate 
feedback when their needs are not met. Moreover, domestic animals and plants 
provide immediate and forceful positive reinforcement when their needs are met. 
There is developing evidence from studies of adult caregivers that providing 
care to others and seeing their needs met is positively reinforcing for the 
caregiver. Caring for others, in short, is good for your health. For children, 
as well as adults, experiencing the positive effects of your care on others, may 
promote feelings of self-efficacy and positive connectedness, which are powerful 
reinforcers. 
Thirdly, we have evidence that nurturing animals and plants may be particularly 
beneficial for boys. This is because caring for babies and young children 
becomes associated in children's minds with "women's work" or "what mommies do" 
as early as 3 years of age and by age 4-5, boys become less interested in 
infants and their care and even avoidant of babycare experiences. However, we 
have found no such association in children's minds when it comes to caring for 
pets or for plants and no sex differences in behavior as children develop. 
Because pet care is "gender-neutral," it may be a particularly useful training 
ground for the development of nurturance in boys. 

Dimensions Of Nurturance
Now that we've seen in a general way why animals and nature may be a positive 
context for the development of nurturance, let's take a more systematic look at 
existing research. To do this, it is useful to distinguish three broad 
dimensions of nurturance. First, we can examine emotion, which may range from 
interest in something out side oneself to desire to meet the developmental needs 
expressed by this other and distress when these needs are not met. Here, we are 
studying children's affect, feelings and emotional investment in animals, 
particularly, pets. 
Second, we can study the dimension of behavior, what skills children have in 
nurturing others and how they apply those skills. Here, we are examining the 
behavioral repertoire children have in relation to domestic animals, especially 
pets and beyond that, how they apply that repertoire to situations of 
developmental needs. This taps the child's responsiveness, his or her ability to 
apply behaviors as appropriate and in response to the needs of the other. With 
respect to pets, plants (and babies), responsivity involves the ability to 
"read" nonverbal cues to assess need and then to select behaviors that will 
presumably meet that need. 
The third dimension is cognition, the ideas and knowledge that children have 
about the object of nurturance, its needs and how to meet them. This dimension 
taps children's ideas about animals and about nature and their thoughts about 
how they can nurture them. 
What do we know about these three dimensions of nurturance as applied to animals 
and to nature? First, with respect to emotion, we have documented in our own 
studies of children from a wide range of family circumstances and social class 
backgrounds that children express strong attachment in general to their 
household pets. This emotional investment is relatively high by the preschool 
period and appears to increase moderately as children get older. (We have 
studied up to age 12.) Despite the overall high levels, there is considerable 
individual variation, so that one may say some children are highly attached to 
their pets, while others are only moderately attached. (Almost no children are 
indifferent.) Children who are highly attached spend more time with their pets, 
doing both caregiving and other kinds of activities, from playing with the 
animal to "confiding" in the animal. Our findings suggest that strength of 
attachment is associated with empathy and positive feelings of competence as 
well as fewer behavior problems at school for children making life transitions, 
such as kindergarten children who have recently made the transition to public 
schooling. For older children, in second and fifth grades, for example, very 
high attachment to pets is not associated with empathy and is sometimes 
negatively associated with feelings of competence. In other words, our findings 
suggest that emotional investment in pets may promote well-being and nurturance 
training (as exhibited by empathy) during times that children are making 
difficult transitions. At other times, when children are expected to be oriented 
more outside the home and toward peers, very high attachment to the pet may be a 
symptom of difficulties in these areas. 
With respect to behavior, we have evidence that children who have pets in the 
home are involved in a range of regular caregiving activities such as feeding, 
walking, grooming, playing, cleaning living quarters, etc. These activities 
involve assessing the needs of the pet and developing skills to meet those needs 
effectively. Boys and girls do not differ in their involvement in these 
nurturing behaviors toward pets, but they do differ in their nurturing behaviors 
toward younger brothers or sisters or toward babies outside the family. Girls 
spend more time caring for younger siblings or other babies than boys do, and 
this gap increases as children approach adolescence. Thus, there is evidence 
supporting the hypothesis that pets (and perhaps other domestic animals) are 
indeed gender neutral avenues for learning nurturance. 
We also have evidence suggesting that pets may be a compensatory avenue for 
nurturing when others are unavailable. Comparing children with both younger 
siblings and pets to children who have pets but no younger siblings, the latter 
spend more time playing with and caring for their pets than the former. With 
respect to cognition, several studies suggest that children with or without pets 
are developing a storehouse of ideas about the developmental needs of common 
household pets and how to meet those needs. Children who have pets in their 
home, however, know more about how adult animals care for their young. And 
children with a stronger emotional tie to their pets have more ideas about what 
their pet is like and how pets may be cared for. In our interview studies, we 
have been struck by the extent of children's knowledge about their pets and 
their care. They are clearly building an "internal working model" of this 
relationship and their role in it. It is not known if this mental representation 
is carried forward to influence other relationships of care, but this is a 
pressing research question. 
In summary, evidence exists that involvement with pets is related to all three 
dimensions of nurturance development. Other aspects of development also may be 
affected by involvement with domestic animals. In providing nurturance to 
another whose needs we meet and who is dependent upon us, we are also 
experiencing a situation of relative power and relative powerlessness. For 
children who are generally dependent and powerless, the experience of dominance 
and power may provide experiences of mastery and self-efficacy. What is less 
recognized is the fact that experiences with animals and nature also provide the 
opportunity to redefine ideas about power and mastery within the context of 
nurturance and care. We can use these experiences of children with animals and 
nature to move from a utilitarian definition of power to an interconnected 
definition of power. 
Secondly, experiences meeting the needs of animals provide unconditional 
positive affect to children. The nonverbal mode of communication not only may 
sharpen the child's ability to decode nonverbal signals (we know that rejected 
children are deficient in this), but also may provide children with needed 
tactile stimulation and "contact comfort." Anthropologists like Edward Hall have 
termed our culture a "noncontact" culture; cross-culturally, our children, 
particularly young children, may be receiving very low levels of tactile 
support. 
Thirdly, involvement with animals and nature may have an evolutionary basis, 
echoing Konrad Lorenz's argument that the "babyish" profile of the young of all 
species innately evokes a nurturing response from humans. There may be 
physiological - that is, hormonal and neurological - changes tied to the sensory 
feedback we receive from the natural environment. We now know that exposure to 
light and dark have profound psychological consequences mediated by their 
sensory impact. Perhaps it is time to launch studies that consider lack of 
involvement with animals and nature as a form of sensory deprivation. 
Many research questions remain. First, does involvement with animals and nature 
affect all children in similar ways? There appear to be age and sex differences 
in the impact of involvement with pets. Should research proceed using a basic 
needs or a deficit model? In other words, is interconnection with animals and 
nature something that all children need at higher levels than they are presently 
getting? Or does such interconnection function in some remedial way to provide 
experiences for children at risk for optimal development?
Second, does involvement with animals and nature function in different ways 
during different developmental periods or life transitions? Are animals 
primarily. "transitional objects" in Winnicott's terms? Do they provide support 
during difficult transitions but become crutches that may interfere with other 
aspects of functioning? 
Third, how does involvement with animals and nature interact with other family 
and contextual variables? No influence on development acts in isolation, 
particularly from family, neighborhood and culture. Animals may provide children 
lessons in cruelty and exploitation if that is how they are interpreted and 
presented. Children may become involved with nature as a force to be tamed and 
destroyed rather than to be valued. Studies of environmental awareness as it 
develops over childhood document that interconnectedness and nurturance are only 
one of the possible ways that children may orient themselves to the natural 
world. Some children develop a more utilitarian, exploitative orientation to the 
environment. It is important to discover the antecedents of these views. 
The importance of family, neighborhood and community means that we must be 
cautious in attributing positive developmental outcomes to the salutary effects 
of animals and nature alone. A positive family environment may amplify and 
reinforce the positive lessons a child receives from involvement with animals. 
It is also possible that involvement with animals may "buffer" the child 
somewhat against family stresses, but what kind of involvement and how much is 
necessary to have an inoculating effect, if indeed any is possible, is not 
known. 
These are but a few examples of the many substantive research issues that 
remain. There also is need for greater methodological rigor in order to document 
what is deeply believed and what many see "works." There is lack of prospective, 
longitudinal and experimental studies (with appropriate control groups) to 
address issues of effects, not just correlations or associations. To say that 
emotional attachment to pets is related to empathy, as we have found, cannot 
imply that pets cause children to be more emphatic, since already emphatic 
children may have the skills to foster deeper attachments to their pets. 
The beneficial effects of pets is a social stereotype, as any look at 
advertising supports. This means we may get socially desirable responses from 
children and adults when we question them about their pets. Hostility, jealousy, 
dominance may not be expressed although felt. There is need for more sensitive, 
non-reactive measures of research on children and pets. 
Despite the need for more and more rigorous research, I think we are ready to 
make two simple but compelling claims. First, we need to promote nurturance in 
our children if we are to live in a more nurturing society. Second, positive 
interconnections with animals and nature are one of the ways in which nurturance 
may develop.

Другие статьи

Анималотерапия

На главную


Hosted by uCoz