Abstract
Objectives To develop a child-friendly methodology to study
children s representations of social support available from
their personal relationships; and to examine children s
representations of support from their pets compared to support
from human relationships.
Design Participants were 22 year-3 primary school children
aged 7-8 years. They were asked to list all the people and
animals important to them and then to select a top 10 of most
special relationships. Using a story-based methodology, children
were asked who from their top 10 they would turn to if they were
the child in the story.
Results Consistency in the data indicated that the children
could reliably discriminate between different relationships in
terms of the support functions they serve. Pets were often
ranked higher than certain kinds of human relationship, and they
featured prominently as providers of comfort, esteem support and
confidantes for a secret. Confidence in these findings is gained
through pets not being nominated for functions they could not
realistically perform.
Keywords: children, pets, social relationships, social
support
Pets are popularly thought to afford special benefits and
enjoyment to children, which may explain why pet ownership is
most frequent in families with children (Rehm 1993). These
presumed benefits include the learning of responsibility for
care, empathy for another living thing, and education in matters
of illness, death and reproduction (Bryant 1990; Davis &
Juhasz 1995; Melson 1998). Pets are also widely believed to
provide a special form of companionship for children, this being
the most frequently stated reason for acquiring a pet for
children (Endenburg 1995).
Research into children and their pets appears to be divided
into two broad approaches: research investigating the
characteristics of the child-pet relationship (e.g. Bryant 1990;
Endenburg 1995); and research comparing child pet owners with
child non-owners and their social development in such social
skills as empathy and support seeking (Covert et al. 1985; Van
Houte & Jarvis 1995). Less attention has been paid to the
role of the pet in a child's wider social network, comparing it
with the roles and functions served by other (human)
relationships. If what goes on between child and pet is like a
social relationship then, for the nature and function of this
relationship to be understood, it needs to be investigated as
part of the child's wider network of social relationships
together with the perceptions the child has of his/her other
relationships.
Traditionally, research into children's relationships has
focused on particular types of relationships such as
relationships with parents, siblings or friends. Typically, each
type of relationship is seen as having a small number of core
features such as security and a secure base in attachment,
conflict and caregiving in sibling relationships and reciprocity
in friendship. However, more recent work indicates that
similarities between different types of relationship are as
striking as their differences. Dunn (1993) provides evidence
that various types of relationships have many features in common.
For example, mutual warmth, expressed affect, conflict, shared
activity, comfort seeking and so on may feature not only in
parent-child relationships but also in child-sibling
relationships, child-friend relationships and many others. The
social-support functions of children's relationships further
illustrate Dunn's point that many relational characteristics and
functions may be present in a variety of different relationships.
To assess these functions, self-report questionnaires have
been developed for use with older children. Furman and
Buhrmester (1985) developed a questionnaire for children aged
11-13 years which assessed both perceived availability and
quality of support over the a range of relationship functions.
The results provide evidence that children can reliably
distinguish between the characteristics and functions of
different relationships in their social networks.
For younger children, the use of self-report questionnaires
has obvious limitations. Reid et al. (1989) suggested that
research on assessing support networks in younger children
should use instruments that are interesting and enjoyable,
interactive, make use of visual materials, and be personally
relevant and commensurate with the child's cognitive and
emotional understanding. Moreover, there should be scope for
investigating both family and non-family relationships.
Reid et al. (1989) devised a methodology called 'My Family
and Friends' for use with children between 6 and 12 years old.
Children were asked to list their family members, relatives,
friends and their teacher, the names of these being written on
individual cards. The children were then asked to rank these in
answer to verbal questions directed to assess the instrumental,
emotional, and companionship support they provide. A card
barometer with a moveable indicator was then manipulated by the
children to indicate the level of satisfaction the children
perceived from the relationships to these forms of support.
This methodology was found to be acceptable and understood by
even the younger participants. It had acceptable test-retest
reliability and demonstrated that children were able to identify
support availability from specified relationships in their
social networks and to give assessments on the satisfaction
received. Reid et al.(1989) distinguished between 'specialist'
and 'generalist' support providers. A generalist is a person who
was viewed by the child as providing a range of support
functions over a number of situations, such as mothers and
fathers, whilst a specialist was a person who was regarded as
being a provider of a particular type or types of support. The
results confirm that even young children can and do distinguish
between relationships in terms of the support they provide.
Research on children's social networks has concentrated on a
limited range of types of relationships. Often, researchers
identify or constrain which types of relationship are to be
investigated rather than permitting the child to select which
relationship they consider to be important, and few studies have
considered the potential role of pets in children's social
networks. Yet recent findings suggest that children may regard
pets as important relationships. Bryant (1985) investigated
social support networks in children between 7 and 10 years by
taking them on a 'neighbourhood walk' designed to incorporate
cues from everyday activity and experience into the interview
schedule and so improve reliability and validity. She found that
children spontaneously generated the names of their pets, or
neighbours' pets, as members of their social networks and
described them as special friends or confidants. The
identification of these two relationship functions had high
test-retest reliability with correlations of 0.99 and 0.92,
respectively. In a later paper, although not comparing support
functions of pets and human relationships, Bryant (1990)
examined the nature and benefits that may accrue to the child
from his/her relationship with a pet. She identified four main
factors: mutuality, a form of close companionship; enduring
affection; self-enhancing affection, probably through elevated
self-esteem; and exclusivity.
The study reported here investigated young children's
perception of support, using a methodology which permitted the
children to nominate their own significant relationships. In so
doing, the children frequently nominated pets, so we were able
to explore children's perception of support available from pets.
The methodology had a lot in common with that used by Reid et al.
(1989), but it allowed the children to directly express
comparisons between different relationships as sources of
support
Participants were a class of 22 children aged between 7 and 8
years (year 3) attending a middle school in a small town in the
English Midlands. Thirteen participants were boys and nine were
girls. Eighteen children owned pets, four did not.
The basic testing procedure involved two stages, which were
carried out on 2 consecutive days. The procedure was repeated 7
days later to assess reliability.
The first stage of the procedure was to obtain a view of each
child's social network as they, the individual child, perceived
it. This was conducted with the class as a whole. The children
were asked to make a list of their friends and their families,
including any pets they may own, their teachers and other people
who may be important to them, such as childminders or family
members who did not reside in their household. No number was
specified as to the size of the list, and help was offered to
children who wished to list a person but were unable to spell
their name or relationship.
When each child had completed his/her list, they were asked
to select the 'top 10' most special relationships. The children
were then presented with a range of materials with which to make
cardboard figures to represent the characters in their top 10
selection.
The second stage of the procedure took place the following
day. Each child was interviewed separately in a room near to the
classroom. The children were asked to show the experimenter
their top 10 figures and to introduce them by name and by
relationship.
The experimenter then told the child that she would tell a
number of short stories. These stories were designed to elicit
the child's responses to a need for support and to identify
those relationships perceived by the child as best providing the
type of support required. The stories examined needs for comfort,
companionship or to feel better about themselves (esteem support).
Additional stories investigated the presence of conflict in
relationships. For each story the child was asked, if they were
in a situation like the child character portrayed in the story,
which of their top 10 relationships they would most like to help
them. Then, if that person were not available, who would they
most like, and so on until they had selected five relationships
(except for the conflict stories when they were only asked for
three choices). The sequence of choices was taken to be the
child's rank order of preference among his/her top 10
relationships for the particular story. The decision not to ask
the children to indicate the order of preference for all of the
top 10 relationships was based on observations during pilot
studies that whilst children found it easy to select the first
four or five choices for most scenarios (except the conflict
stories), they frequently remarked that, although the remaining
relationships were special, they would not approach any of them
for that function described in the story. This was even more
marked in the conflict scenarios for which children commonly
nominated fewer relationships.
Participants were interviewed individually and eight short
stories were told. Each story was accompanied by a storyboard,
comprising pictures and words, which helped the child focus on
the main character and the type of need he/she required. The
dialogue was conducted using the cardboard figures of the
child's top 10 as props to help the child indicate a chosen
relationship, and to indicate to the child when one of the
characters was not available. The stories can be summarized as
follows.
The subject was shown a storyboard depicting a small boy in
bed. The subject was told that the boy was poorly and could not
go out to play and would have to stay in bed until he was better.
As he was feeling a bit fed up and miserable, he would like
someone to come to see him and help cheer him up a bit.
The subject was then asked 'If you were ill in bed, and you
felt like he (the boy in the story) felt, who would you most
want to come and see you?' The subject then had to choose one of
his/her figures to represent who he/she would most want to come
to see them if they were ill in bed. When the subject selected
his/her first choice, the experimenter then removed the figure
and said, 'But what if X (selected character's name) wasn't
there, who would you choose?' The subject then selected their
next choice. The procedure was repeated until five characters
had been selected and were recorded by the experimenter as ranks
one to five in the subject's choices. The characters were then
returned to the subject in readiness for the next story.
The second story also examined the need for comfort, this
time in the context of having to cope with a potentially
frightening event. The subject was shown a storyboard and told
of a small boy who had to walk home from school through a long
dark lane. The boy thought it was very creepy and would have
liked someone to be with him to stop him feeling scared. The
subject was again asked 'If that was you, and you had to go down
a long dark lane, who would you most want to be with you?' The
subject then had to select his/her first choice of person in
their top 10 to accompany them to stop them feeling scared. This
first choice was then removed and the experimenter asked, 'If X
wasn't there, who would be the next best?' This continued until
five choices had been made from the available 10.
The first of two esteem-related stories concerned a girl who
had been selected to take a big part in a school play. It was a
difficult part and she had to practice lots of songs, her words
and actions. She makes lots of mistakes while she is practising
at school and at home. This makes her feel silly and embarrassed.
She knows she needs to practice to make sure she is ready for
the play, but she only wants to practice in front of people who
won't make her feel silly or embarrassed if she does make
mistakes. The subject was asked who they would choose as being
best to practice in front of, if they had to do what the girl in
story does. The procedure of removing each choice, as before,
was continued for all the remaining scenarios.
The second esteem-related story told of a girl who is
normally very sensible and organized, but who had a 'bad day'
when lots of things went wrong. She was late for school, forgot
her pencil case so that the teacher was annoyed, forgot her
lunch, lost things and then falls over on the way home from
school and ruins her best shoes. Everyone says 'Oh, you silly
girl!' all day long and she feels very bad about herself because
this is so unlike her normal behaviour. She needs someone who
knows she is not really silly and who would be able to reassure
her that it was just one of those days. The subject was then
asked who would be most likely to be able to make them feel that
they were not silly really.
The first story on confiding examined the telling/sharing of
a special secret. The subject was told a story of a young girl
who found a magic door at the bottom of her garden. The door led
into a wonderful magic garden. The girl wishes she had someone
to share in the excitement. A friendly wizard suddenly appears
and welcomes her, but also warns her that she may only share her
secret with one person. If she told any more than one other
person, she would never find her way back into the magic garden.
The subject was then asked who they would share the secret with,
if they were ever in that situation.
This story depicted a small boy who was being bullied by
children he did not know. It was making him very miserable and
presenting quite a problem to him. He knows that he has to tell
someone who can help him. The subject was asked to imagine that
they were in that situation, and who they thought would be most
helpful to tell of their problem.
A miniature plastic dustbin was placed in front of the
subject. He/she was told that although people/pets can be
special, we all get annoyed with them at times. When someone is
especially annoying we can put them in the bin! The subject was
asked, 'Who annoys you so much that you would put them in the
bin first?' The subject made his/her first selection. They were
then asked 'Who would be next in the bin because they annoyed
you almost as much?' This exercise was limited to three choices
only since many children indicated that only a minority of their
relationships warranted being put in the bin for annoying them.
A second conflict scenario was presented to examine the
subjects' perception of who they thought found they themselves (the
subject) annoying. Subjects were asked 'Who do you think finds
you annoying? Who would be the first to want to put you in the
bin?' The subject selected the character they thought would be
most likely to put them in the bin for being annoying. Again,
the process was repeated for the selection of three characters.
The whole procedure, except for he construction of the first
network list, was repeated one week later to test the
reliability of the subjects' responses. On retest, subjects were
told that the exercise should not be regarded as a memory test
and that they should choose their top 10 relationships from
their larger lists as honestly as they could, even if this meant
they differed from their first selection.
The mean number of relationships generated by the children
for their larger first list of all the relationships considered
important to them was 15 (range 9-23), so for the majority (18)
of children there was scope for their top 10 to differ between
the two occasions. Of these 18 participants, 12 had perfect
agreement on their choice of top 10; the mean value of kappa
coefficients of agreement was 0.89. These findings indicate good
test-retest reliability for the selection of top 10 lists of
relationships, which implies that the children did not make
their selections merely on the basis of temporary preferences
prevailing at the time of testing.
Eighteen participants owned pets and listed these on their
larger overall list. Of these children, 17 included one of more
pets in their top 10: nine participants had either a cat or a
dog in their top 10; four had a 'small pet' (rabbit, hamster,
guinea pig or budgerigar) in their top; four had a cat or dog
and also a small pet to their top 10. The child who did not
include his pet in his top 10 owned a guinea pig. Some
participants who owned more than one pet included them on the
overall list but selected only one for their top 10 selecting,
for example, a budgie out of a budgie and a hamster; a rabbit
from a rabbit and a dog; and a dog from a dog and a guinea pig.
This willingness to select pets as significant relationships
supports Bryant's (1985) claim that children may regard pets as
special friends and companions within a child's social network.
To examine the reliability of rankings of relationships, for
each of the eight stories rankings from the first session were
correlated with rankings from the retest session. The
correlations on the diagonal of Table I are the test-retest
correlations for each story. The high level of these
correlations indicates that children are relatively consistent
across sessions in their choice of relationships.
Correlations between different stories (in the off-diagonal
cells in Table I) were generally lower than the test-retest
correlations. Thus, in general, the situations featuring in
different stories elicited different rank orderings of people
whom the children perceived as most suitable sources of support.
However, a few stories achieved moderately high correlations
with other stories, most strikingly confiding a problem and
support of self-esteem after a bad day. This indicates a similar
choice of people nominated for their perceived value in
providing help or support in these two situations. Other
examples are comfort when scared and confiding a problem, and
support in an embarrassing situation and confiding a problem, so
those three scenarios tend to associate into a cluster.
Intuitively, similarities in rankings between scenarios makes
sense since children were selecting from their own limited
choice of 10 special relationships who could be expected to give
a range of support functions in differing situations.
However, some of the stories revealed that children hold
quite firm, consistent views about the appropriateness of
relationship functions to be obtained from different people. The
most noticeable of these were the two stories concerning
conflict which each had very low correlations with other stories,
despite having high test-retest correlations. The story on
comfort when ill had moderately high correlations with support
for self-esteem after a bad day, but other correlations
involving comfort when ill were generally low. Confiding a
special secret also tended to have low correlations with other
support scenarios. These low correlations suggest that the
choice of people for providing supportive functions was
different from one story to another.
These findings indicate that children can and do make
considered selections of the appropriateness of people within
their social network to fulfil relationship functions in
accordance with a need presented by situations and that, child
by child, their selection of appropriate people is consistent.
Further analysis was conducted to examine whether the
children were similar to one another in their selection of
particular types of relationships for particular needs. Although
the children had slightly differing selections of relationships
in their top 10 lists, many of the nominated relationships were
similar. For example, all children nominated mother and father,
siblings, best friends and at least one other family member such
as an aunt or a grandparent. The 17 relationship types listed in
Table 2 were sufficiently common across the sample to permit
analysis. The category 'small pets' comprised guinea pigs,
rabbits, hamsters and budgerigars.
To measure the degree of consistency in the way that the
children ranked these relationships in response to the stories,
intraclass correlation coefficients were computed (ICC(2,k) in
the terminology of Shrout & Fleiss 1979). With one exception,
these coefficients were substantial (range 0.56-0.90) and
statistically significant (P < 0.01) for both session 1 and
session 2 data. The exception was for story 3: self-esteem in an
embarrassing situation which, although significant for session
1, fell just below statistical significance for session 2 (ICC =
0.4). Overall, there was a high degree of consistency across
children in which particular kinds of relationships they
selected for different stories. It would appear that children
share similar perceptions both of the needs portrayed by the
different stories and the appropriateness of particular
relationships to elicit particular functions.
Table 2 shows which relationships were among the five most
likely relationships to be selected for each story (based on
mean ranks). Viewed by column this shows which relationships
were most frequently selected for each story; viewed by row it
shows how many stories each relationship was favoured for.
Younger siblings are ranked highly only in one or both of the
stories concerning conflict, and seem not to be regarded as
appropriate people from which to elicit help with a problem,
comfort when ill or in a frightening situation, or to bolster
esteem. This is much as would be expected. Older siblings are
also highly ranked for both of the conflict situations, being
seen as both annoying to the subjects and likely to perceive the
subjects as a source of annoyance. Older brothers are also seen
as appropriate sources of help in situations where a child may
feel scared, perhaps as a function of them being an older person
to accompany the child. Similarly, perhaps because of the nature
of the problem described in the story in which a child was being
bullied, older brothers were nominated as being of potential
help. Older sisters were not regarded as appropriate for either
of these stories.
Best friends were most likely to be the person with whom
subjects would choose to share a special secret. Again, this is
much as might be expected, as is the presence of best friends as
high ranking in all but the conflict stories. Teachers figured
only as potential helpers with a problem such as bullying and as
sources of annoyance to subjects. Mothers were consistently
ranked highly for all stories except that of being annoying to
subjects. Fathers were less likely to be selected as appropriate
for providing comfort when ill, or sharing a special secret, but
as more appropriate for providing help when the child was scared,
needed a boost to self-esteem or had a problem.
Some relationships nominated as being part of the top 10 list
were not among the relationships most likely to be selected for
any of the stories. These tended to be relationships that were
not immediate family, such as uncles, aunts, and grandparents.
In contrast, some relationships featured amongst many stories.
This was especially so in the case of mothers, fathers and best
friends and indicates a closeness of relationship that fulfils
many needs across many situations. This reflects what Reid et al.
(1989) referred to as generalist providers of support.
Relationships fulfilling a narrower range of supports, or
regarded as appropriate for only certain situations, are termed
'specialist' providers. Examples of specialist providers in this
sample are teachers, as helpers with a bullying problem, and
friends (not best friends) with whom a subject may share a
secret.
Rankings given to pets suggest that children can
realistically discriminate between functions that pets can or
cannot provide. For example, no pets were nominated as being
able to help with a bullying problem. Dogs were seen as a useful
protector in a scary situation whereas cats were not. Perhaps
because dogs are more active play companions than cats, dogs
were also ranked highly for sharing a special secret. Small pets
achieved high rankings only for self-esteem in an embarrassing
situation-as non-judgemental relationships in front of whom a
child could rehearse or practice and not feel silly if they made
mistakes
However, the functions perceived as available from cats and
dogs strongly suggests that children regard these as potential
sources of comfort and/or esteem. Cats featured particularly
highly as providers of comfort if a child was ill in bed. Dogs
were also regarded as comforters when ill (although not as
highly as cats) but were especially perceived as serving a
supportive function in a frightening situation, and as playmates
with whom to share a special secret. This would appear to
suggest that pets play a specialist role in children's support
networks that, although not confined to the provision of just
one function, is limited by the child's appreciation of the
animals' limitations. Provision of comfort, esteem and play
appear to be the dominant roles for these pets.
It is also striking that cats and dogs consistently achieved
higher rankings than many of the participants' human
relationships, notably non-immediate family members such as
aunts, uncles and grandparents. This would indicate that pets
are regarded as close family members not only through residing
in the same house, but through the functions they provide.
The aims of this study were to examine the significance of
pets in children's social networks and the functions they may
serve as perceived by the children, using a methodology designed
to place pets in a context of the roles and functions available
from human relationships in the children's social networks.
The methodology was found to be highly satisfactory in that
it enabled children to generate their own choices of
relationships, whereas other studies into relationship function
that have often used relationship types prescribed by the
researchers. The results leave little doubt that children are
able to both identify special relationships and to discriminate
between them to obtain appropriate support if needed. The
methodology is therefore well suited to children who are too
young for the use of self-report measures. The stories and
illustrated story-boards were much enjoyed by the children (especially
the stories concerning putting annoying people in the bin).
Asking the children to make a sequence of 'best vs. the rest'
judgements seemed more appropriate than asking them for a
straight ranking of the relationships.
The results confirm the findings of Reid et al. (1989) that
children's social networks contain both generalist and
specialist support providers. In this study, generalists were
identified as parents and best friends, and specialists as
teachers and friends. Siblings were also identifiable as
specialist providers of mutual annoyance and conflict although,
with special regard to older siblings (especially brothers),
there were possible functions as protectors or helpers with a
problems.
The results also confirm the claims of Bryant (1985) that
pets may assume significant relationships in children's social
networks, although this was only apparent for cats and dogs, and
add to our understanding of children's representations of these
relationships. Cats were especially seen as comforting when a
child was ill, whereas dogs were regarded as comforting during
illness and supportive if the child were scared. Dogs were seen
as companions/playmates for special ventures. Thus it would
appear that dogs and cats offer special relationships for
provision of psychological forms of support (i.e. making one
feel better about oneself) but not for the more practical
problems a child might have to deal with. The fact that cats and
dogs frequently ranked higher than many human relationships
suggests the value that children place on their pets and the
functions they serve, and that studies examining children's
networks would be advised to include any pets that may be owned.
This research was carried out while the authors were in
receipt of financial support from the Waltham Centre for Pet
Nutrition. June McNicholas is a Waltham Research Fellow. The
authors thank Emma Myring for collecting the data.
Accepted for publication 13 September 2000
Legend for Chart:
B - Comfort when ill
C - Comfort when scared
D - Self-esteem (embarrassed)
E - Self-esteem (bad day)
F - Confiding a secret
G - Confiding a problem
H - Conflict: (annoys you)
I - Conflict: (you annoy)
A B C D E
F G H I
Comfort when ill 0.49 0.22 0.25 0.41
0.24 0.29 -0.09 0.02
Comfort when scared 0.12 0.48 0.33 0.32
0.34 0.31 -0.14 0.18
Self-esteem (embarrassed) 0.10 0.19 0.43 0.27
0.27 0.32 -0.06 0.14
Self-esteem (bad day) 0.34 0.30 0.45 0.57
0.25 0.51 -0.21 0.18
Confiding a secret 0.27 0.29 0.27 0.21
0.54 0.18 -0.04 0.05
Confiding a problem 0.16 0.46 0.45 0.57
0.24 0.63 -0.14 0.25
Conflict: (annoys you) -0.14 -0.05 -0.19 -0.24
-0.11 -0.12 0.66 0.25
Conflict: (you annoy) 0.15 0.21 0.11 0.24
0.08 0.25 0.30 0.81
The correlations on the diagonal of the table are
test-retest correlations for the same story across the two
sessions. The correlations in the off-diagonal cells are between
different stories, either in session 1 (in the upper-right part
of the table) or in session 2 (lower-left). The correlation
coefficients were computed as pooled within-subjects correlations
with 104 degrees of freedom (based on 22 subjects and five
observations per subject). We suggest r = 0.3 as conservative
criterion for statistical significance.
Legend for Chart:
A - Relationship
B - Story Comfort when ill
C - Sotry Comfort when scared
D - Story Self-esteem (embarrassed)
E - Story Self-esteem (bad day)
F - Story Confiding a secret
G - Story Confiding a problem
H - Story Conflict: (annoys you)
I - Story Conflict: (you annoy)
A B C D E F G H I
Mum * * * * * * *
Dad * * * * *
Best friend * * * * *
Brother[O] * * * * *
Dog * * *
Sister[Y] * *
Cat *
Cousin * *
Brother[Y] *
Sister[O] * *
Friend *
Teacher * * *
Aunt
Grandma *
Grandad
Small pet *
Uncle
[O] = older sibling
[Y] = younger sibling.
References
Bryant, B. K. (1985) The Neighbourhood Walk: Sources of
Support in Middle Childhood. Monographs of the Society for
Research in Child Development, 50, 1-116 (Serial no. 210).
Society for Research in Child Development, London.
Bryant, B. K. (1990) The richness of the child-pet
relationship: a consideration of both benefits and costs of pets
to children. Anthrozoos, 3, 253-261.
Covert, A. M., Whiten, A. P., Keith, J. & Nelson, C.
(1985) Pets, early adolescents and families. Marriage and Family
Review, 8, 95-108.
Davis, J. H. & Juhasz, A. M. (1995) The preadolescent pet
friendship bond. Anthrozoos, 8, 78-82.
Dunn, J. (1993) Young Children's Close Relationships: Beyond
Attachment. Sage, Newbury Park, CA.
Endenburg, N. (1995) The attachment of people to companion
animals. Anthrozoos, 21, 83-89.
Furman, W. & Buhrmester, D. (1985) Children's perceptions
of the personal relationships in their social networks.
Developmental Psychology, 21, 1016-1024.
Melson, G. F. (1998) The role of companion animals in human
development. In: Companion Animals in Human Health (eds C. C.
Wilson & D. C. Turner), Sage, Thousand Oaks, CA.
Rehm, N. (1993) Dog keeping in the family. In: Proceedings of
the International Congress on Applied Ethology, Berlin 1993 (eds
M. Nichelmann, H. K. Wierenga & S. Braun), Humboldt
University, Berlin.
Reid, M., Landesman, S., Treder, R. & Jaccard, J. (1989)
'My family and friends': six-to twelve-year-old children's
perceptions of social support. Child Development, 60, 896-910.
Shrout, P. E. & Fleiss, J. L. (1979) Intraclass
correlations: uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychological
Bulletin, 86, 420-428.
Van Houte, B. A. & Jarvis, P. A. (1995) The role of pets
in preadolescent psychosocial development. Journal of Applied
Developmental Psychology, 16, 463-479.
~~~~~~~~
By J McNicholas, Department of Psychology University of
Warwick Coventry CV4 7AL UK and G M Collis, Department of
Psychology, University of Warwick, Coventry, UK
|